Current issues of ACP Journal Club are published in Annals of Internal Medicine


Editorial

Assessing allocation concealment and blinding in randomized controlled trials: Why bother?

PDF

ACP J Club. 2000 Mar-Apr;132:A11. doi:10.7326/ACPJC-2000-132-2-A11



The scientific community’s quest for unbiased research received a strong boost from a recent policy amendment on randomized controlled trials (RCTs) in this journal. Henceforth, the status of allocation concealment will be clearly indicated in the abstracts along with that of blinding, so readers will have additional information by which to judge the internal validity of trials. In this editorial, I address the background of and rationale for these enhancements.

Background

Random allocation to intervention groups remains the only method of ensuring that the groups being compared are on an equivalent footing at study outset, thus eliminating selection and confounding biases. This technique has allowed RCTs to play a key role in advancing medical science.

The success of randomization depends on 2 interrelated processes (1, 2). The first entails generating a sequence by which the participants in a trial are allocated to intervention groups. To ensure the unpredictability of that allocation sequence, investigators should generate it by a random process. The second process, allocation concealment, shields those involved in a trial from knowing upcoming assignments. Without this protection, investigators and patients have been known to change who gets the next assignment, making the comparison groups less equivalent (3-6).

For example, suppose that an investigator creates an adequate allocation sequence using a random number table. However, the investigator then affixes the list of that sequence to a bulletin board, with no allocation concealment. Those responsible for admitting participants could ascertain the upcoming treatment allocations and then route participants with better prognoses to the experimental group and those with poorer prognoses to the control group, or vice versa. Bias would result. Inadequate allocation concealment also exists, for example, when assignment to groups depends on whether a participant’s hospital number is odd or even or on translucent envelopes that allow discernment of assignments when held up to a light bulb.

Allocation concealment should not be confused with blinding. Allocation concealment concentrates on preventing selection and confounding biases, safeguards the assignment sequence before and until allocation, and can always be successfully implemented (1, 2). Blinding concentrates on preventing study personnel and participants from determining the group to which participants have been assigned (which leads to ascertainment bias), safeguards the sequence after allocation, and cannot always be implemented (1-7).

Reporting of methods

Investigators must not only minimize bias but must also communicate those efforts to the reader. Readers should not have to assume or guess the methods used. Yet assessments of the reporting quality of published trials have consistently found major flaws (3, 8-14). Only 9% of trials in the specialist journals and 15% in the general journals reported both an adequate method of generating random sequences and an adequate method of allocation concealment (3, 8, 15). Of trials reported as double blind, only 45% described similarity of the treatment and control regimens, and only 26% provided information on the protection of the allocation schedule (16). Most reports simply provide no information on methods.

With so little relevant information, many of us resort to inappropriate markers of trial quality. 2 noteworthy examples are described here. First, many designate a trial as high quality if it is “double blind,” as if double blinding is the sine qua non of an RCT. Although double blinding can reflect good methods, it is not the sole criterion of quality. As I shall discuss later, adequate allocation concealment actually appears to be the more important indicator. Moreover, many trials cannot be double blinded. Those trials must be judged on other merits and not on an inapplicable standard based on double blinding.

Second, some assume that a good-quality trial contains groups of equal size, while a poor-quality trial contains groups of unequal size. That standard applies only when the investigators use a restricted randomization generation scheme that aims for equality. A simple randomization method will seldom yield equal sample sizes. In fact, equal numbers in treatment groups may mean that some process other than randomization was used, for example, allocation of every second patient to the intervention group or the use of odd and even birth dates or chart numbers as a way to assign participants to study groups.

Although RCT reporting remains weak, it is improving. Methodologists, editors, and clinicians addressed the prevailing flaws in reporting by publishing the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement (17). Currently, more than 70 journals have adopted the standards, including such high-profile general medical journals as JAMA, Lancet, BMJ, and Annals of Internal Medicine. Yet, even with improvement, readers of RCTs should be wary of the information provided in many current trial reports.

Empirical evidence of bias

Recent studies have shown that poor-quality RCTs and poorly reported RCTs yield biased results. For example, in a study of 250 controlled trials from 33 meta-analyses in pregnancy and childbirth, investigators found that alleged RCTs with inadequate and unclear allocation concealment yielded larger estimates of treatment effects (41% and 33%, respectively, on average) than trials in which authors reported adequate concealment (5). Investigators found similar results for trials in digestive diseases, circulatory diseases, mental health, and stroke (18). Those trials that used inadequate or unclear allocation concealment yielded 37% larger estimates of effect, on average, than those that used adequate concealment.

These exaggerated estimates of treatment effects reveal meaningful levels of bias. If a study is designed to detect a decrease in mortality of 25% or 50% from a particular treatment, biases of 30% to 40% would overwhelm estimates of the treatment effect. The elimination of bias is crucial in trials designed to detect moderate effects.

Double blinding also appears to reduce bias. Trials that were not double blinded yielded larger estimates of treatment effects than did trials in which authors reported double blinding (odds ratios exaggerated, on average, by 17%) (5). Another recent analysis has also noted the importance of double blinding (19). However, although double blinding appears to prevent bias, its effect appears weaker than that of allocation concealment. Indeed, Moher and colleagues found little effect from double blinding (18).

Conclusions

As users of RCT results, we must understand the potential for humans to interject bias. By including assessments of allocation concealment and double blinding, abstracts in this journal will help readers to discern those trials that have made superior efforts to minimize bias. Judging the quality of allocation concealment and blinding reflects current empirical research and reflects the commitment of the editors of this journal to apply the principles of evidence-based medicine to the practice of reporting.

Kenneth F. Schulz, PhD, MBA
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
Atlanta, Georgia, USA


References

1. Schulz KF. Subverting randomization in controlled trials. JAMA. 1995;274:1456-8.

2. Schulz KF. Randomized trials, human nature, and reporting guidelines. Lancet. 1996;348:596-8.

3. Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Grimes DA, Altman DG. Assessing the quality of randomization from reports of controlled trials published in obstetrics and gynecology journals. JAMA. 1994;272:125-8.

4. Chalmers TC, Levin H, Sacks HS, et al. Meta-analysis of clinical trials as a scientific discipline. I: Control of bias and comparison with large co-operative trials. Stat Med. 1987;6:315-28.

5. Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Hayes RJ, Altman DG. Empirical evidence of bias. Dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. JAMA. 1995;273:408-12.

6. Pocock SJ. Statistical aspects of clinical trial design. Statistician. 1982;31:1-18.

7. Schulz KF. Unbiased research and the human spirit: the challenges of randomized controlled trials. CMAJ. 1995;153:783-6.

8. Altman DG, Dore CJ. Randomisation and baseline comparisons in clinical trials. Lancet. 1990;335:149-53.

9. Moher D, Fortin P, Jadad AR, et al. Completeness of reporting of trials published in languages other than English: implications for the conduct and reporting of systematic reviews. Lancet. 1996;347:363-6.

10. Williams DH, Davis CE. Reporting of assignment methods in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials. 1994;15:294-8.

11. Sonis J, Joines J. The quality of clinical trials published in The Journal of Family Practice, 1974-1991. J Fam Pract. 1994;39:225-35.

12. Mosteller F, Gilbert JP, McPeek B. Reporting standards and research strategies for controlled trials: agenda for the editor. Control Clin Trials. 1980;1:37-58.

13. DerSimonian R, Charette LJ, McPeek B, Mosteller F. Reporting on methods in clinical trials. N Engl J Med. 1982;306:1332-7.

14. Tyson JE, Furzan JA, Reisch JS, Mize SG. An evaluation of the quality of therapeutic studies in perinatal medicine. J Pediatr. 1983;102:10-3.

15. Schulz KF, Chalmers I, Altman DG, et al. The methodologic quality of randomization as assessed from reports of trials in specialist and general medical journals. Online J Curr Clin Trials [serial online]. 1995 August 26; 4(Doc No 197);[81 paragraphs].

16. Schulz KF, Grimes DA, Altman DG, Hayes RJ. Blinding and exclusions after allocation in randomised controlled trials: survey of published parallel group trials in obstetrics and gynaecology. BMJ. 1996;312:742-4.

17. Begg C, Cho M, Eastwood S, et al. Improving the quality of reporting of randomized controlled trials. The CONSORT statement. JAMA. 1996;276:637-9.

18. Moher D, Pham B, Jones A, et al. Does quality of reports of randomised trials affect estimates of intervention efficacy reported in meta-analyses? Lancet. 1998;352:609-13.

19. Khan KS, Daya S, Collins JA, Walter SD. Empirical evidence of bias in infertility research: overestimation of treatment effect in crossover trials using pregnancy as the outcome measure. Fertil Steril. 1996;65:939-45.